Diversity and InclusivenessRacism's Guts? The Physiology of Oppression

Racism’s Guts? The Physiology of Oppression

by Shannon Sullivan

Can a person’s belief in racial hierarchy—for example, that white people are superior to Black people—be embodied in their gut? I wondered this after having an “aha” moment while reading Elizabeth Wilson’s book Psychosomatic: Feminism and the Neurological Body. Chapter two of her book examines the psychology of the stomach and the intestines and, in particular, their role in severe depression experienced by a gay man as he confronted homophobia. It hit me that the gut, and the body more generally, are far more “mentally” sophisticated than I had imagined and that this sophistication is not reserved for the brain. Maybe the social-political world literally helps structure and constitute human physiology, such that the effects of both domination and oppression can be found in people’s bodily functions.

In a nutshell, that is what my book, The Physiology of Sexist and Racist Oppression, is about, but I didn’t know how to develop the idea at the time. And my spouse gave me a strange look confirming that the idea was, at best, half-baked. At worst, as I worried, the idea sounded oddly racist, as if I were saying in a confused fashion that race and/or being a racist is determined by biology. And if there is one thing about race that everyone is supposed to know by now, it’s that it is not biological. It is a social construction. On this view, race and racial categorization are a set of social beliefs (including mistaken ones about biology) that are backed by institutional structures and public policies, but there is nothing biologically real about them. If we didn’t realize that before 1950, the United Nations Educational, Scientific, and Cultural Organization (UNESCO) made it perfectly clear at that time. In the wake of World War II, experts assembled by UNESCO issued a 1950 Statement on Race that declared that there is no biological basis for race and thus no scientific grounds for racial categorization or racism. Associating race and biology was what the Nazis did to justify Jewish genocide. Disassociating race and biology thus was a necessary way to fight Nazism, anti-Semitism, and all other forms of racism. The only way to talk about race and biology—unless you are a Nazi or a white supremacist—supposedly is to insist that they have nothing to do with each other. (The irony here is that after 1950, the medical sciences generally continued to operate as if race were biologically real, sometimes by talking about “populations” and population genetics, rather than “races.” But maybe it’s not so ironic after all. Racism and white supremacy are incredibly tenacious, which is a big part of what this book is about. It is going to take a lot more than conscious, rational argumentation to eliminate racism, white supremacy, and their offshoots, and we are fooling ourselves if we think otherwise.)

As for my half-baked idea, I still thought there was something important about it, but I kept it to myself for a while. Wilson’s book doesn’t discuss race or racism, but it showed me how social-political situations can literally shape a person’s physiological functions and, in turn, how a person’s physiological functions can be an unconscious way of responding to the world. What could that mean about a world—our world—which is suffused with race, racism, and white privilege, I wondered? What could that mean about the physiological constitution of people’s bodies—my body—as a white woman in that world?

I argue in the book that while race is not biological in a static or ahistorical way, it becomes biological in and through racist oppression. In a related fashion, it argues that sex/gender categories of male and female are not biologically given or determined, yet sex/gender becomes biological in and through sexist oppression. This happens in an intersectional fashion. This means that a person’s race is not separate from and then added onto her gender. Race and gender instead dynamically inform and co-constitute each other—as do other socially relevant aspects of our identities, such as sexuality, religion, nationality, class status, and more. In the United States and many other nations, for example, to become a white woman generally means something different physiologically than it means to become a Black woman.

But what does that mean? And why is it important to think about? For starters, it means that the oppressive effects of racism and sexism are going to be even harder to eliminate than we likely thought. And it’s important to realize this so that we don’t overlook a key place that racism and sexism have put down their roots. Very tough roots, it turns out. Even if we could snap our fingers and magically eliminate all racism and sexism today—all racist and sexist beliefs, as well as all racist and sexist institutions, policies, and structures—racism and sexism would not disappear. Their effects would endure in our bodies for years, even for generations. For example, they would endure in the shorter telomere length that African American women have on average in comparison with white women of the same age. Telomeres are the ends of chromosomes, and they shorten over time as the body wears down. This produces a person’s physiological age, which might not be the same as their chronological age. Two women could have been born in the United States in 1969, and in 2019 they would have the same chronological age of 50. However, on average, an African American woman’s shorter telomere length means that her physiological age is 57 ½ while that of the white woman is 50. This difference wasn’t present or biologically mandated when the two women were born five decades ago. But it is part of their different biological make-up at age 50, contributing to the lower life expectancy for African American women than white women in the United States.

Wilson’s book also led me to read more about the gut. It turns out that biologists and medical and health scientists have known since the early 1990s that sexual assault tends to fundamentally change people’s gut functioning. “People” here typically means women, given the higher rates of sexual assault experienced by women than by men. And “gut functioning” is not a metaphor. It literally means the gastrointestinal system: how the tissues, muscles, and fluids of the stomach and intestines process—or not—the food and liquid that is put inside them. The gut can take on a different character depending on the patterns and habits of its functioning, and women and men tend to have different gut characters because of the frequency of sexual assault in our world. “Character” here is not metaphorical. I am talking literally about the psychological and moral qualities distinctive to an individual and how those are shaped by gender in a sexist and male privileged world. Those qualities are found in a person’s gastrointestinal tract, which can “tell stories that people cannot or will not tell, either because they are unable, forbidden, or choose not to tell,” as epidemiologist Nancy Krieger has argued. It’s no coincidence, for example, that women suffer from disproportionately high percentages of functional gastrointestinal disorders, such as Irritable Bowel Syndrome (IBS) and Crohn’s disease. (“Functional” means that the disorder is not caused by a diseased or damaged organ, e.g., from a severe virus or a car wreck.) The gut is a human being’s largest external—yes, external—surface through which we take in the world, and women’s guts often tell us that they want to spew it out.

A person’s character also can be found in other aspects of their physiology, such as their hips, pelvic floor, heart functioning, and epigenome. The main chapters of Physiology focus on these areas of the body, and one chapter in particular follows up on my initial idea that white persons’ stomachs could be racist. I find the epigenome particularly interesting because of its connections to genetics. Our society has been fixated on genes for a while, culminating in The Human Genome Project, which fully mapped the human genome between 1990-2003. The public understanding of the project—perhaps also the scientists’ too—was that once we fully understand all of the genes that make up a human being, we’ll be able to predict and cure diseases as well as know why a person acts the way that they do. (The fixation on finding a “gay gene” has been particularly intense, in my view.) But genes aren’t the entire story. They don’t exist apart from an epigenome, which is the set of chemical modifications to DNA and various proteins that regulate the expression of a gene. The epigenome is fundamentally impacted by both physical and social environments. Think of the epigenome as like a knob that can turn the “volume” of a gene completely up or down or somewhere in between. A person could have a particular gene, but it might not be expressed (“turned up”) at all depending on environmental factors. In that case, it would be as if the person didn’t have the gene. In addition, in some cases the environmental effects on a person’s epigenetics can be passed down from one generation to another.

Epigenetics can help us understand racist disparities in health for African American women, particularly their high pre-term birth rates, which often persist across generations. I said “racist” rather than “racial” since it is racism, not merely race that produces the health disparities. The daily stressors of racism and white privilege that take a physiological toll on an African American person’s telomeres, for example, also can impact their epigenome. In the case of African American women, environmental stressors play a significant role in whether genes associated with premature labor are expressed. While everyone can and does have stress in their lives, there is something else going on in the case of African American women. “There is something about growing up as a black female in the United States that’s not good for your childbearing health,” as neonatologist Richard David explained, “I don’t know how else to summarize it.” The impact of this “something” not only affects the genomic health of an African American woman, moreover. It also can be epigenetically passed onto her daughters and granddaughters via transgenerational epigenetic inheritance. While such inheritance is more common for plants than mammals, there exists evidence that direct epigenetic inheritance occasionally occurs for mammals too. What we are learning is that the harmful effects of racism, white privilege, and white supremacy are very biologically durable. They not only can alter the health of one generation, but it is possible that they also are passed down transgenerationally.

As is probably obvious, Physiology is not a feel-good book. Racism and sexism are far more intractable than we’ve probably imagined. That isn’t necessarily cause for despair. It does, however, point to the need for specific strategies to cope, endure, and perhaps even thrive in ongoing situations of oppression. Those strategies can and should include not only changes in health policies, but also forms of embodied movement and physiological therapy (as distinguished from “physical therapy”) to transform biologically unconscious habits, as the book’s conclusion discusses. If there is one thing that I hope readers take away from the book, which perhaps is somewhat uplifting, it is an appreciation of the human body’s amazing psychophysiological complexity. We often mistakenly assume a very biologically flat understanding of the body, as Elizabeth Wilson has claimed, when in fact our biologically unconscious habits are tremendously psychologically dynamic and multidimensional. This makes the physiological body an important site for feminists, critical race scholars, and others who care about social justice to do their work. Biology isn’t the entire story, of course, and it needs to be approached not just sympathetically, but also critically. However, we discard it at our peril given the need to learn more about how racism and sexism constitute our unconscious habits. Those habits are thoroughly physiological, which means that we have a lot to learn about the stories that our bodies can tell.

Shannon Sullivan is Chair of Philosophy and Professor of Philosophy and Health Psychology at UNC Charlotte. She is author or editor of nine books, including Race and Epistemologies of Ignorance (2007), Good White People: The Problem with Middle-Class White Anti-Racism (2014), The Physiology of Sexist and Racist Oppression (2015), and White Privilege (2019).

12 COMMENTS

  1. Thank you for this post, Shannon! I am so grateful to continue learning from your work, and have struggled to think about practices of holding responsible in the face of your insights about how racism manifests in the body. I’d love to hear more about the notion of physiological therapy (which is totally new to me), and look forward to reading your book!

    • Thanks Katie! I admit I made up the notion of physiological therapy 🙂 or rather the ideas I was working on led me there. It’s somewhat brief, but I talk about it in the conclusion of the Physiology book. I’d like to do more with the concept hopefully in connection with some of my colleagues who work in biobehavioral health (in my university’s Health Psychology program). You might also find helpful Bessel van der Kolk’s The Body Keeps the Score, which I highly recommend.

  2. Thanks for the interesting post! Is there any evidence that racism affects The racists’ physiology as well as those who are discriminated against? Privilege (especially if it is less stressful) might do it but does being racist (as opposed to just privileged because, say, one is white) affect one’s physiology?

    • Hi Nicole,
      Hmm, that is a really interesting question, and I think that most of the arguments/evidence I have on this are more related to white privilege than to racism. However, in many cases I think it’s hard to cleanly separate those two, and I do think & would want to argue that if the definition of racism that we are working with includes emotionally felt beliefs–vs. “purely” cognitive ones, if those even exist for human beings–then racism does affect a racist person’s physiology. I’m also thinking here of the vignette I open with in the Physiology book, of a white woman who claimed her stomach clinched up when she saw black men as she was walking alone on the street and that this was proof that black men were threatening/scary. She wouldn’t say she was a racist; it was just that her stomach reacted that way to a “real” (read something like: objective) situation in the world. I would want to talk about her situation as one in which racism (in the form of believing that blackness and especially black men = criminality) has shaped the gut reactions of a white women … and indeed, shapes the gut reactions of many white women, not just her, if we are going to be honest about it. So I wouldn’t say that she necessarily was consciously racist, but that she unconsciously held racist beliefs that were expressed via her stomach muscles, acid production etc.

  3. The claim that unconscious habits are thoroughly physiological contradicts empirical evidence. Tiger have claws and fangs because they are hunters and ruminates have several stomachs because they are grazers Hence, it is self-evident that psychology gives rise to physiology, not vice versa.

  4. The order of physiology and psychology ultimately resolves into that of “having” and “being”. The above article argues that “having” precedes “being” which is deterministic thus denying the possibility of creativity, morality, self-motivation and even original thought. In that case, the issues of racism and sexism are emotional not rational and political not scientific. This needs to be recognized and accepted if intelligence is to prevail. Obfuscation is not the solution.

    • Hi John,
      I’m unsure how to reply to your two posts except that to say that you are right that I want to push to consider a lot of things as physiological that we usually don’t consider that way — emotions in particular. Also I would argue that issues related to emotion (such as racism and sexism, but not only those) are very relevant to science and scientific discussion. That would not, on my view however, necessarily make them apolitical.

      • Hi Shannon,
        I understand your intention and my confusion arise from that. To assist you in responding, I elaborate as follows:

        My first point is that “being” precedes “having” per my example of tigers and ruminates. As additional examples, promiscuity can result in sexually transmitted infections and type-A personalities are prone to heart-attacks, stomach ulcers, etc. Are you suggesting that these health issues cause the associated personality traits?

        I mention (in passing) that any attempt to substantiate that Whites/males are racist/sexist because they are less healthy than Africans/females might reveal that they are, in fact, more healthy. Has that possibility occurred to you? Also, if Whites/males are claimed to be more prone to racism/sexism than Blacks/females (for whatever reason – physiological, psychological, rational or irrational) wouldn’t that claim itself be racist/sexist? If so, have you not fallen into the logical trap of Epimenides, the Cretan, who claimed all Cretans always lie?

        Also, as explained, the claim that psychology is attributable to physical causes leads to determinism which denies the possibility of creativity, morality, justice, self-motivation, free choice, original thought, etc. Scientific rationalization thus objectifies Self but surely that is contrary to any humanistic stance that condemns racism/sexism?

        Finally, if race and gender are dismissible as social constructs, racism/sexism cannot be condemned on moral/political grounds because they are also social constructs and equally dismissible. Alternatively, if the issue is economic (unequal pay), it is also moral/political because based on justice and the same inconsistency applies. Do you agree that this inconsistency is unscientific?

        Hopefully you are now better able to understand the reasons for my confusion and request for clarification. I look forward to hearing from you.

        • Hi John,
          I think there’s more here than I can respond to in a blog post, so I’ll take up your comment about “being” and “having” because I think it might be at the heart of some of our disagreements and/or help us figure out if we are indeed disagreeing. (I touched on this in my other response to a later post of yours, so this probably is more of an elaboration than a new point.) As I analyze racism & race and sexism & sex/gender, I wouldn’t characterize it as an issue of being in relationship to having. One of the key arguments of the book is that the “is” in the statement “race is physiologically [or biologically] real” is an “is” of becoming, not being. Racial differences of, say Black and white, become physiologically real differences as the effects of racism, white supremacy, and related social factors. On my view, racial differences are neither “merely” social constructs nor eternal or ahistorically necessary, but they are very real and persistent/resistant (which doesn’t mean they are unchangeable…but it will be a lot harder to change them than I think we generally have reckoned with to this point). Another way of putting this is that the account of human reality that I’m using/arguing for is one that is biopsychosocial and doesn’t divide up biology vs. society or being vs. processes and activities (“having”?). And I’m trying to do that in very concrete ways connected to health issues and the health sciences rather than just in the abstract.

  5. The claim that race is a social construct is somewhat puzzling because, if that is so, what about morality? If both are social constructs, it is irrational to condemn racism on moral grounds because morality is equally dismissible.

    Language is a social construct that defies scientific analysis but does that make it immoral to recognize and accept that languages are not all the same and make value judgments in that regard?

    • Hi John,
      Actually the Physiology book doesn’t claim that race is a social construct, but it also doesn’t claim that it is biological in the typical way that that claim is made. It argues that the social construct vs. biologically real dichotomy is a false one and that race becomes biological through the physiological impact of racism and white privilege/white supremacy. Likewise, I argue that sex/gender become biological through the physiological impact of sexism and male privilege. So-called social constructs are very real, on my view, with very real bodily effects that shouldn’t be dismissed. They can be changed, but they are extremely durable so we shouldn’t expect that they will be easy to change “just because” they are generated through social practices (both individual/community and institutional/structural)..

      • Hi Shannon,
        You seem to be saying that superiority/inferiority is a belief not a fact but the belief brings about the fact. In other words, the difference between oppressor/oppressed or superior/inferior is that between positive and negative thinking.

        If the above interpretation is correct, it accords with Nietzsche’s description of the difference between the master and slave mentality which I attribute to reverse understandings as to the order of cause and effect.

        To the superior, the present is the cause of the future (conceptual) whereas, to the inferior, the present is the effect of the past (perceptual). Hence, the difference between oppressor and oppressed is that between “being” (conceptual) and “having” (perceptual).

        To the superior, “being” is the cause and “having” the effect but, to the inferior, that order is reversed. I do not believe this difference is intentionally contrived but understand it to be a fact of nature as with sporting talent such as tennis or golf.

LEAVE A REPLY

Please enter your comment!
Please enter your name here

WordPress Anti-Spam by WP-SpamShield

Topics

Advanced search

Posts You May Enjoy

Reflections on My Undergraduate Experience in Philosophy

In my first year at Queen’s University (Ontario, Canada), I had originally planned to study psychology in the hopes of becoming a therapist. I...